A critique of an academic research article on business management

A critique of an academic research article on business management Order Description Select a recent research journal article that presents the results of empirical research in an area that you are interested in. You must do this in week 1 to share with your seminar group and obtain approval that it is suitable from your seminar tutor. You should title your review as follows: A critical review of……author(s), date of publication, title of article, name of journal, volume and issue number. The critique should address the following tasks and questions: Why are you interested in reading this research article and how will it help you in your research? Summarise in your own words what the article is about. Is it clear what the research questions or issues are? What are the main theories and literature quoted? Comment on the way in which the evidence (data) was obtained. Evaluate the main arguments used by the author(s) Evaluate the evidence put forward to support these arguments. What contribution does this research make to knowledge and management practice? Journal of Marketing Management Vol. 28, Nos. 9–10, August 2012, 1174–1189 Using response behaviour theory to solicit survey participation in consumer research: An empirical study Felicitas Evangelista, University of Western Sydney, Australia Patrick Poon, Lingnan University, Hong Kong, PRC Gerald Albaum,University of New Mexico, USA Abstract The purpose of this article is to report on research that examines survey participation rates (i.e. response rates) for personal interview surveys where solicitation for participation is based on different theories of survey-response behaviour in two culturally distinct countries. Field experiments were designed to investigate the extent to which the theories of exchange, cognitive dissonance, self-perception, and involvement/commitment can influence potential respondents to participate in a personal interview survey in Australia and Hong Kong. The results show that there were significant differences in Australia with the theory of self-perception having the strongest impact on survey-response behaviour, while cognitive dissonance has the least impact. In contrast, the effects in Hong Kong were not significant. This study adds to the limited empirical research regarding why consumers participate in surveys, particularly personal interview surveys. The theories are applied at the self-introduction and invitation to participate, which is a crucial stage in the potential respondent’s decision about participation. Keywords theories of survey-response behaviour; personal interview surveys; survey response rates; consumer research; field experiment Introduction Extant research on survey participation in consumer research may be grouped into two categories. The first consists of studies aimed at identifying ways by which response rates could be increased, and the second is focused on finding an explanation for why consumers participate or do not participate in surveys. Between these two types of studies, the former tends to dominate the literature. It is of common knowledge to researchers in this area that there are a great number of empirical studies on response inducement techniques (see Martin, Duncan, & Powers, 1989) and other methodological artefacts that affect response to surveys, particularly ISSN 0267-257X print/ISSN 1472-1376 online © 2012 Westburn Publishers Ltd. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0267257X.2011.619148 http://www.tandfonline.com Evangelista et al. Using response behaviour theory to solicit survey participation 1175 mail surveys (Gendall, Hoek, and Esselmont, 1995) but increasingly Internet (web) surveys. The paucity of studies focusing on personal interview surveys (Lynn, 2001; Shaw, Bednall, & Hall, 2002) or modified personal interview surveys (East & Uncles, 2008; Webster, 1997) and on theory testing and development has led to the focus adopted in this article. This article reports the results of a field experiment involving different appeals used to solicit survey participation. The appeals represent the application of four theories in explaining the decision to participate or not participate in a personal interview survey. Conducted in two countries with diverse cultures (Hong Kong and Australia), this study builds on the earlier work of Albaum, Evangelista, and Medina (1998), Evangelista, Albaum, and Poon (1999), and Poon, Albaum, and Evangelista (2003). A related study is that by Webster (1997) who examined the effect of type of appeal on response quality in hand-delivered, self-administered surveys. The appeals used – social utility, help-the-sponsor, and egoistic – can be viewed as being based on a theory of response behaviour. In addition to testing different solicitation appeals based on the theories of social exchange, cognitive dissonance, self-perception, and involvement/commitment, this article also aims to determine their effectiveness in different cultural settings. The next section of this article is devoted to a discussion of relevant theories of survey response, which is followed by a description of the study’s research design, results, and conclusion. Theoretical framework In developing a theory of survey participation, it is important to understand the interaction between respondent and interviewer, and the behaviour, attitudes, and expectations each brings to the interaction in a survey environment (Groves, Cialdini, and Couper, 1992). The focus on the interaction at the self-introduction or invitation stage is particularly critical. In a study conducted by Groves and Couper (1998) involving face-to-face interviews, it was found that many survey design features have their effect because they make favourable consequences of the interview more salient to the respondent prior to his/her decision to participate or not participate. Somewhat related is the recent study by Tourangeau and Ye (2009) in which an experiment was conducted in a random-digit-dialing telephone survey. For one half of the respondents, the request for participation emphasised the benefits of participation, and for the others the emphasis was on the loss that would be involved if they chose not to participate. Based on prospect theory (Kahneman & Tversky, 1979), it was predicted that the loss framing would be more effective than the gain framing. Results confirmed the prediction, although the difference was slightly less than 10 percentage points in the response rate. A recent study by Powers and Valentine (2009), using a mail survey, showed that, in a satisfaction survey, the degree of interest a consumer has in the subject of the survey can influence respondent responses and completion rates. Personal contact surveys have distinct advantages. Compared to telephone or mail surveys, personal contact is much more flexible, allows for the collection of a greater variety of data, and is better when questionnaires are lengthy (Webster, 1997, p. 105). One further advantage is that response can be obtained from identified specific individuals, thus reducing, or even eliminating, problems due to respondent integrity behaviour (this behaviour is discussed in Braunsberger, Gates, & Ortinau, 2005). 1176 Journal of Marketing Management, Volume 28 The specific consequences that should be emphasised could depend on the major reason why a potential respondent would participate in a survey. In searching for these reasons, four major theories have been suggested in the literature as being applicable. The three most cited that are relevant to marketing research are exchange (Dillman, 2000), cognitive dissonance (Furse & Stewart, 1984; Hackler & Bourgette, 1973), and self-perception (Allen, Schewe, & Wijk, 1980; Tybout & Yalch, 1980). In addition, Albaum (1987) has suggested that a theory of involvement or commitment might also fill the gap in theory development and use by marketing researchers. Each of these theories is described below. Exchange Social exchange theory asserts that the actions of individuals in answering a questionnaire are motivated by an expected return or reward, which may not necessarily be monetary in nature. The occurrence of a behaviour such as responding to a survey would be a function of the perceived cost in engaging in that activity and the expected rewards (Tullar, Pressley, & Gentry, 1979). Three conditions need to be present in order for survey response to be maximised: (1) the costs for responding must be minimised; (2) the rewards must be maximised; and (3) there must be a belief by potential respondents that such rewards will, in fact, be provided. The guidance provided by exchange theory is, however, only general (Dillman, 2000). Many of the techniques used to induce compliance can be viewed as being designed to provide rewards, lower costs, and establish trust. For example, if completing a questionnaire or responding to an interviewer’s questions can be made to be a rewarding act, the process itself may provide the motivation to participate in the survey. An implication for the researcher, therefore, is to make the questionnaire as interesting as possible (Dillman, 2000). Another example is that of token financial incentives. Their effectiveness is probably not so much due to their monetary value as it is to their being a symbol of trust. Evidence supporting the positive impact of incentives is provided by Lynn (2001) who reported for a personal interview survey a significantly higher cooperation rate from sample members who were provided incentives compared to those who were not. Cognitive dissonance Cognitive dissonance theory (Festinger, 1957) provides a mechanism for integrating, within a single model, much of the empirical research that has been done on inducement techniques for survey response (Furse & Stewart, 1984). As used for explaining survey response, the theory postulates that reducing dissonance is an important component of the ‘respond/not respond’ decision of potential survey respondents. In a mail survey, and likewise an Internet-based survey, a questionnaire with a cover letter asks for participation (Furse & Stewart, 1984). For telephone and personal interview surveys, the process is started by a verbal request for participation. Failure to respond to a survey will produce a state of dissonance, which the potential respondent seeks to reduce by becoming a survey respondent. The dissonance model has had limited tests. Hackler and Bourgette (1973) attributed the high response rate they found for a dollar inducement to the ability of the money sent to induce cognitive dissonance in potential respondents. Furse, Evangelista et al. Using response behaviour theory to solicit survey participation 1177 Stewart, and Rados (1981) found that the initial monetary incentive served to create dissonance, but that repeating the same incentive in a follow-up mailing did not induce dissonance among those not responding initially. Apparently, there are people who are dissonance resistant. These researchers felt that dissonance explained the success they found regarding the foot-in-the door technique and that it would also explain some of the negative findings experienced when that technique was used. To some extent, cognitive dissonance is meant to be a motivational state following behaviour when there may be insufficient justification for the behaviour (Brehm & Cohen, 1962). Until behaviour such as not responding to a request for survey participation occurs, the state of ‘respond/not respond’ is conflict that is relatively rational. In the present study, however, we view the so-called conflict the same as dissonance for the ‘respond/not respond’ situation, as a potential respondent can worry about ‘Will I make the right decision?’ This allows us to be consistent with how dissonance theory has been applied in methodological research in the field of marketing. Self-perception Self-perception theory asserts that people infer attitudes and knowledge of themselves through interpretations made about the causes of their behaviour (Bem, 1972). Interpretations are made on the basis of self-observation. To the extent that a person’s behaviour is attributed to internal causes and is not perceived as due to circumstantial pressures, a positive attitude towards the behaviour develops. Thus liking something is really observing one’s own positive behaviour towards the object. In the same way, a person infers that another person likes an object by observing their behaviour. These attitudes (i.e. self-perception) then affect subsequent behaviour. As mentioned by Helgeson, Voss, and Terpening (2002) in a study of mail-survey response, self-perception denotes that individuals are more likely to engage in behaviour when that behaviour is consistent with pre-existing attitudes. Self-perception would predict that labelling one’s behaviour would cause that person to view herself or himself as the kind of person who engages in such behaviour, therefore the likelihood of later label-consistent behaviour is increased (Tybout & Yalch, 1980). Self-perception theory has often been applied in survey research to explain the effects, or lack of effects, associated with the foot-in-the-door technique (DeJong, 1979). This technique attempts to elicit participating in a study by first gaining compliance with a small request, such as answering a couple of questions in a telephone pre-call. According to Allen et al. (1980), self-perception can be operated as follows: ‘. . . responding with a small behaviour (answering the phone interviewer’s questions) should alter one’s self-perception (I am a participator) and influence subsequent behaviour (filling out and returning the mail questionnaire)’. Involvement/commitment Somewhat related to the theories presented above is the theory of involvement or commitment (Becker, 1960). Commitment leads to involvement. The concept of commitment has been used by sociologists to explain consistent behaviour. It can be defined as ‘a variable which encompasses the ranges of allegiance an individual may be said to have for the social system of which he is a member’ (Hornback, 1971, p. 65). Characteristics of this so-called consistent behaviour are that it (1) persists over some period of time, (2) leads to pursuit of at least one common goal, and 1178 Journal of Marketing Management, Volume 28 (3) rejects other acts of behaviour (Becker, 1960). Consequently, the major elements of commitment are viewed as including: • The individual is in a position in which her/his decision regarding a particular behaviour has consequences for other interests and activities not necessarily related to it. • The person is in that position by her/his own prior behaviour. • The committed person must recognise the interest created by one’s prior action; even though one has such an interest, she/he will not act to implement it unless it is realised as being necessary. A person who is highly committed to some activity is less likely to terminate the activity than one who is uncommitted (Ford, 1973). This concept of commitment or involvement is consistent with what consumer behaviourists have called enduring involvement – contrasted to situational involvement (Havitz & Howard, 1995; Houston & Rothschild, 1978). Enduring involvement reflects a sustained level of concern with an issue, product, or activity and, as such, remains stable over long periods of time. In contrast, situational involvement is situation specific. The theory of commitment (or involvement) can be extended to explain survey-response behaviour in that commitment can be attached to many different aspects of a survey, such as the source or the sponsor, the researcher, the topic and issues being studied, and/or the research process itself, and this can lead to involvement through participation in a survey. The study by Brennan and Hoek (1992) seems to suggest that enduring commitment would seem to exist. However, situational commitment is not precluded. Potential respondents may have varying feelings of commitment to the various aspects attached to a survey. It is postulated that this theory may help to explain survey-response behaviour in considering the ‘respond/not respond’ decision. The theory of commitment does not suggest that there will always be consistent behaviour regarding response to surveys in general, although this is not precluded. Rather, the theory suggests that there would be consistent response behaviour for people facing the same type of survey under the same situation and conditions. The theory of commitment has been used very little to explain methodological effects. For example, attempts to apply this theory have involved studies of (1) the effects of inducement questions, that is, an initial question to arouse respondent interest (Geurts, O’Neill, Lawrence, & Albaum, 1988), and (2) the effects of survey source and coding in mail surveys (Albaum, 1987). Propositions to be tested The preceding discussion of theoretical frameworks of survey-response behaviour forms the basis for developing different appeals to solicit survey participation. As there is no definite theory that could help predict which appeal would generate the greatest response, this study’s main proposition in its null form is as follows: P1: There will be no difference in response rates due to the solicitation methods developed along the theories of self-perception, social exchange, cognitive dissonance, and involvement/commitment. Evangelista et al. Using response behaviour theory to solicit survey participation 1179 The effects of culture The effects of culture on survey response patterns have been investigated in previous studies. These studies have found cultural differences in terms of social acquiescence, extreme response, social desirability, and topic bias (Johnson & Van de Vijver, 2003; Lyness, & Kropf, 2007; Skjak & Harkness, 2003). Acquiescence and response styles have been found to be significantly affected by country-level characteristics such as power distance, collectivism, uncertainty avoidance, and extraversion (Harzing, 2006). However, to the best of our knowledge, there have been no studies about the direct effects of culture on response rates per se. In view of this, we could only draw limited insights from cross-cultural psychology and consumer behaviour studies. One such study by Watkins et al. (1998) relates to the theory of self-perception. In this study, self-esteem ratings among adults and children were found to be higher among Western Europeans than Asians. Other studies involving physical and global self-perception rating comparisons between British and Hong Kong samples showed similar results (Hagger, Biddle, Chow, Stambulova, & Kavussanu, 2003). The difference is attributed to the ideological background of the respondents’ or participants’ cultural group along the individualism/collectivism continuum (Bond & Smith, 1996). The study was conducted in two quite different cultural settings. Since one’s culture might affect how one responds to a request for survey participation, we can expect differences in response to the differential requests. However, due to nonexistent prior research on the issue, we cannot predict which country would yield the greater response. We therefore state the following in null form: P2: There will be no difference in the response rates between Australian and Hong Kong respondents in terms of the different survey solicitation appeals. The effects of solicitation appeal on data quality In general, methodological comparison of different survey solicitation appeals will be guided by modifying the paradigm for comparing survey methods suggested by Roster, Rogers, Hozier, Baker, and Albaum (2007, p. 134). As shown in Figure 1, which was first proposed by Albaum and Peterson (1985) to study response to mail surveys, major measures of effect are data quality and quantity (i.e. response content), response rate, and cost. As indicated in Figure 1, another effect that needs to be addressed is data quality. Does the solicitation appeal have an effect on data quality? Again, due to a lack of empirical research and theory, we present the following proposition in null form: P3: The solicitation appeals will have no effect on data quality. Methodology Field experiments were conducted in Australia and Hong Kong. The cultures of the two countries are quite distinct, as shown in Table 1. Using Hofstede’s (2001) schema, the only dimension in which the two cultures are close is masculinity/femininity. Both areas tend towards dominant male role patterns. Also shown in Table 1 are 1180 Journal of Marketing Management, Volume 28 Figure 1 A two-stage research paradigm for comparing survey sample and responses. (1) Subsample Comparisons 1. Individual Treatment Variables (Main Effects) 2. Treatment Combinations (Interaction) Raw Data No Difference Significant Difference Adjust Responses and Combine Sample Adjust Separately (Segment Sample) (2) Analysis of Dependent Variable (Measure of Effect) Data Quality and Quantity Cost Response Rate Item Omission Response Content Evaluation and Conclusions Research Design and Manipulation Evangelista et al. Using response behaviour theory to solicit survey participation 1181 Table 1 Differences between Australian and Hong Kong respondents. Measure Australia Hong Kong Hofstede schemaa Power distance 36 68 Uncertainty avoidance 51 29 Individualism/collectivism 90 25 Masculinity/femininity 61 57 Long-term/short-term orientation 31 96 Psychic distance 1b 36.43 8.31 Psychic distance 2c - 8.06 Psychic distance 3d - 20.5 aSource: Hofstede (2001, p. 500). bSource: Ellis (2008). The distances are how far away each is from China. cSource: Brewer (2007). Australia is the base. dSource: Kogut and Singh (1988). Australia is the base. psychic distance measures for the two countries by Brewer (2007), Ellis (2008), and Kogut and Singh (1988). Psychic distance is a measure of how close countries are in terms of many things, including culture, history, and economic and industrial development. Psychic distance is measured at the individual level by assessing the individuals’ perceptions of differences (Sousa & Bradley, 2006, 2008). The four theories described in the previous section were operationalised in the form of introductory scripts that the interviewer used in inviting potential respondents to participate in the survey. These scripts were stated after the interviewer introduced the topic of the survey as below (words in parentheses refer to the Hong Kong scripts): Suppose amarket research interviewer approaches you in a shopping mall and says, ‘Good morning/afternoon. I am ______ from _______. I am conducting a survey on people’s attitudes towards Australian-made (Chinese-made) and foreign-made products. Exchange: We are offering you a $3 scratch lottery ticket (McDonald’s set meal coupon) for your participation. Would you please answer a few questions? Self-perception: We would like to get the opinion of helpful people like yourself. Would you please answer a few questions? Commitment/involvement: The topic of this survey is very important. Would you please answer a few questions? Cognitive dissonance: I hope that you would be able to answer a few questions, as non-participation would cause us great difficulties. Would you please answer a few questions?’ Similar scripts were used by Poon et al. (2003) in their study of Hong Kong consumers, and were found to represent the theories involved. Obviously, other 1182 Journal of Marketing Management, Volume 28 wordings could be used to represent each theory. In addition, the scripts could have been expanded in length to represent the theory more fully. This, however, was not deemed advisable, as the mall intercept mode of data collection was used to obtain the data, and the request for participation should be as short as possible. After the request for participation was made, respondents were asked whether they would participate or not, why they would or would not participate, and certain demographic characteristics (e.g. age, gender, etc.). In Australia, those who said they would participate were then asked to complete the survey regarding Australian-made and foreign-made products. The questionnaire for this survey included seven Likertscale items from the Consumer Ethnocentrism Scale (CETSCALE; Shimp & Sharma, 1987). In addition to the CETSCALE items, questions were asked about preference for specific types of Australian-made products and whether they had purchased any in the past two years. The interviews were conducted by mall intercept in major shopping centres in Sydney, Australia, and Hong Kong. Bush and Hair (1985) compared mall intercept with telephone interviewing and concluded that the mall intercept was a unique form of face-to-face interviewing with much to offer the market researcher. The major concern in a mall intercept survey is sampling the correct respondents. But, with proper training and the use of qualifying questions, the interviewer can handle this with some ease. In the present study, the only qualification was that a respondent be an adult (university student age as a minimum). University students, who completed a thorough training session, conducted the personal interviews. The selection of respondents was left to each interviewer, who used a judgemental sampling of those in the mall. There was no attempt to fill quotas by gender or age. Thus there was a type of randomness in the sampling. When the interviewer completed an interview, he or she would ask the next adult-looking person (university student age or older) passing by to participate. After agreeing, respondents were shown a dummy questionnaire. The dummy questionnaire was similar in both countries, the only difference being the reference to Australian-made and Chinese-made products in the respective countries. In Australia, the interviews were conducted in English, and the dummy questionnaire was in English. In Hong Kong, the interviews were conducted mainly in Chinese, although the dummy questionnaire was bilingual (English and Chinese). A proper translation and back translation process was performed for the bilingual questionnaire used in Hong Kong to ensure the equivalence of questionnaires used in the two countries. In fact, English is still widely spoken and understood in Hong Kong. The study was conducted as a field experiment. The scripts were applied in a rotational fashion such that the ‘exchange script’ was used on the first respondent followed by the ‘self-perception script’ on the next respondent and so on. The script was changed each time a potential respondent listened to the complete script whether he/she decided to participate or not. This process was repeated until the desired total sample size (100 for each script in Australia and approximately 50 in Hong Kong) was reached. Those who refused before the self-introduction/invitation for participation was made or who did not listen to the entire script were excluded from the sample. Finally, a total of 400 Australian respondents and 206 Hong Kong respondents provided the data necessary for analysis. Evangelista et al. Using response behaviour theory to solicit survey participation 1183 Results and findings The profile of the respondents, that is, those who participated in the experiment, is shown in Table 2. No significant relationship was found between the scripts and the different demographic characteristics, that is, age, gender, and education, indicating that the allocation of scripts to the potential respondents was random and unbiased. Given the nature of the experimental design used, the major measures of effect analysed are response/participation rate and data quality (response content). Cost was not assessed in this study, as there is only one mode of data collection. Response (participation) rate The results of the field experiments are summarised in Table 3. In Australia, the script that yielded the highest response rate is self-perception, while the least effective is cognitive dissonance. In Hong Kong, exchange theory is the most effective, and the least effective is self-perception. The chi-square statistics show that in Australia, the type of appeal used has a significant effect on the response rate, while in Hong Kong Table 2 Profile of respondents participating in the experiment. Characteristics Australia N = 400 Hong Kong N = 206 Gender Female 55% 62.1% Male 45% 37.9% Age <30 62.4% 62.1% 30–40 17.1% 19.4% >40 20.5% 18.5% Years of formal education <10 11% 0% 10– 12 38% 46.7% >12 51% 53.3% Table 3 Results of survey participation. Australiaa Hong Kongb Theory Participate Not participate N Participate Not participate N Z p Cognitive dissonance 35% 65% 100 38% 62% 53 .04 <.48 Commitment/ involvement 46% 54% 100 40% 60% 50 .69 <.26 Exchange 48% 52% 100 42% 58% 48 .68 <.26 Self-perception 61% 39% 100 36% 64% 55 3.06 <.01 a?2 = 13.66; p < .005; b?2 = .355; n.s. 1184 Journal of Marketing Management, Volume 28 this is not the case. The lack of significance in Hong Kong could be due to the limited sample size or it could be due to the collectivist nature of the culture in Hong Kong. Moreover, the amount of incentive for the testing of exchange theory might not be sufficient to induce motivation to participate. In contrast, Australian culture is individualistic in nature. Nonetheless, if these results are taken to be indicative of why people participate in surveys, it is evident from Table 3 that a difference does exist between the two countries. The theory that yielded the biggest response-rate differential between the two countries (25 percentage points) is self-perception, whereas the least difference (3 percentage points) emerged for cognitive dissonance. With respect to the individual theories, self-perception is the only one where there is a significant difference in participation between Australia and Hong Kong, based on the difference-in-proportions test (twotailed test). This contrasting result may be attributed to the cultural factors cited earlier. Cultures that are more collectivist in nature, such as Chinese societies, tend to downplay the role of the self in determining behaviour. They tend to emphasise the role of self from a social rather than personal context (Hagger et al., 2003). In the context of the current study, self-perception may have less influence on surveyresponse behaviour in collectivist cultures like Hong Kong compared to a more individualistic culture such as Australia. Data quality (response content) Since there was a significant difference in response rates for the tested theories in Australia, it is difficult to assess whether one theory is better than another without examining whether content of a survey is affected. Since it is very difficult to determine which content would be most valid, if content varied, ideally, a researcher would like to have content not be affected. That way, choice of a theory to design a survey could be made on the basis of other criteria, such as cost, time, and so on. Overall, slightly more than 50% of those participating in the experiment (n = 210) indicated they would respond positively to the survey request. Slightly more than 50% of these respondents were female, the modal age category was <30 years, about 36% were married, and the median income category was A$20,000–$35,000. These respondents completed the questionnaire about attitudes towards Australianmade products. The seven scale items from CETSCALE were used to analyse effect on content or data quality. These items are five-category Likert scale of agreement. Mean values for these items for the four theories are shown in Table 4. Using analysis of variance, significant differences at p < .05 (two-tailed test) emerged for only one scale, ‘Australians should always buy Australian-made products instead of imports’. The overall summated score differences were not significant (p < .16). It is also noted that, psychometrically, there was no difference, as coefficient alphas ranged from .90 to .93. Respondents in Australia were also asked to answer the question ‘How interesting is the topic of the survey to you?’ A five-point numerical scale was the response format, where 1 = ‘not interesting at all’ and 5 = ‘very interesting’. The mean values for the individual theories were not statistically significantly different (F = 1.254, p < .30, two-tailed test). The overall mean value for the entire sample is 3.34. Evangelista et al. Using response behaviour theory to solicit survey participation 1185 Table 4 Mean values of CETSCALE items, by survey response theory#. Theory CETSCALE items SPa SEb CDc C/Id F p 1. Australians should always buy Australian-made products instead of imports. 3.02 2.98 2.74 3.48 2.93 .035* 2. Only those products that are unavailable in Australia should be imported. 3.19 2.98 2.79 3.39 2.03 .111 3. Purchasing foreign-made products is un-Australian. 2.46 2.20 2.44 2.70 1.66 .176 4. It is not right to purchase foreign products. 2.45 2.37 2.00 2.60 2.45 .065 5. There should be very little trading or purchasing of goods from other countries unless out of necessity. 2.79 2.85 2.42 2.93 1.29 .280 6. Foreign products should be taxed heavily to reduce their entry into Australia. 2.71 2.76 2.55 2.98 .97 .410 7. We should buy from foreign countries only those products that we cannot obtain within our own country. 3.00 2.84 2.68 3.18 1.41 .241 Summated score 19.29 18.91 18.07 21.33 1.79 .150 Coefficient alpha .92 .91 .90 .93 # Items are scored 1 = ‘strongly disagree’ and 5 = ‘strongly agree’. aSelf-perception: number of respondents varies from 54 to 60. bSocial exchange: number of respondents varies from 44 to 49. cCognitive dissonance: number of respondents varies from 30 to 34. dCommitment/involvement: number of respondents varies from 42 to 44. *p < .05. Discussion and conclusions A basic premise of this study is that response behaviour theory can be used to design consumer research studies such that response rates are higher without adversely affecting data quality. Moreover, a key dimension is the initial appeal or request for participation that is made to a potential respondent. The effects of four theories – 1186 Journal of Marketing Management, Volume 28 exchange, cognitive dissonance, self-perception, and involvement/commitment – were examined in a field experiment conducted in two distinct cultures, Australia and Hong Kong. Three propositions, presented in null form were tested: P1: No difference in response rates due to solicitation methods based on the different theories. P2: No difference in response rates based on solicitation methods between Australian and Hong Kong respondents. P3: Data quality is not affected by solicitation appeals. Overall, the result of testing Proposition 1 is that it is rejected for Australia but supported for Hong Kong. In Australia, response rates varied from 35% to 61%. Selfperception generated a much larger rate than did the other three theories. In contrast, in Hong Kong, response rates varied from 58% to 64%. For Proposition 2, it is rejected only for self-perception theory, where the difference in participation rates is greatest in Australia. Proposition 3 is supported for five of the seven CETSCALE items and the overall summated score. The findings of this field experiment suggest that survey-response behaviour has a cultural dimension that researchers need to take into account. A standardised selfintroduction and invitation to participate will not work equally well in all cultures. Different appeals may have to be employed when conducting surveys in different cultures. This proposition, however, needs further investigation, as the sample of the current study is small, the samples are from only two cultural groups, and only one topic was studied. Further field experiments involving larger samples from diverse cultures with varying topics are therefore highly recommended. In addition, the extent to which the specific wording of each script in this study can generalise to other modes of data collection, other topics, and other countries/cultures also needs to be examined in further research. In addition to the wording of the introductory scripts, the specific mode of data collection – mall intercept – may be a limitation. The theories may work differently in an exit interview, street-intercept, and an interview in the home, as well as in a mail survey, telephone survey, or web survey. Another potential limitation is that respondents were asked hypothetically about participation. When actually being queried, they may react differently than when faced with a hypothetical situation. In this study, the survey was conducted by personal interview. Finally, the study was conducted in Hong Kong and Australia. Respondents in other countries and cultures may react differently. From a cultural perspective, effect of each theory of survey response on response rate may very well be what Berry (1980, pp. 11–13) has called a cultural emic (i.e. culture-bound). References Albaum, G. (1987). Do source and anonymity affect mail survey results? Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 15(3), 74–81. Albaum, G., Evangelista, F., & Medina, N. (1998). The role of response behaviour theory in survey research: A cross-national study. Journal of Business Research, 42(2), 115–125. Evangelista et al. Using response behaviour theory to solicit survey participation 1187 Albaum, G., & Peterson, R.A. (1985). A paradigm for methodological research on mail survey response. In R. Lusch et al. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 1985 Summer Educators Conference (pp. 363–368). Chicago: American Marketing Association. Allen, C.T., Schewe, C., & Wijk, G. (1980). More on self-perception theory’s foot technique in the pre-call/mail survey setting. Journal of Marketing Research, 17(4), 498–502. Becker, H.S. (1960). Notes on the concept of commitment. American Journal of Sociology, 66(6), 32–40. Bem, D.J. (1972). Self-perception theory. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology. New York: Academic Press. Berry, J.W. (1980). Introduction to methodology. In H.C. Triandis & J.W. Berry, (Eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology: Methodology (Vol. 2, pp. 1–28). Boston: Allyn and Bacon. Bond, M.H., & Smith, P.B. (1996). Cross-cultural, social and organizational psychology. Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 205–235. Braunsberger, K., Gates, R., & Ortinau, D.J. (2005). Prospective respondent integrity behaviour in replying to direct mail questionnaires: A contributor in overestimating nonresponse rates. Journal of Business Research, 58(3), 260–267. Brehm, J.W., & Cohen, A.R. (1962), Explorations in cognitive dissonance. New York: Wiley. Brennan, M., & Hoek, J. (1992). The behaviour of respondents, nonrespondents, and refusers across mail surveys. Public Opinion Quarterly, 56, 530–535. Brewer, P.A. (2007). Operationalizing psychic distance: A revised approach. Journal of International Marketing, 15(1), 44–66. Bush, A.J., & Hair, J.F., Jr. (1985). An assessment of the mall intercept as a data collection method. Journal of Marketing Research, 22(2), 158–167. Dejong,W. (1979). An examination of self-perception mediation of the foot-in the-door effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 2221–2239. Dillman, D. (2000). Mail and Internet surveys: The tailored design method (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley-Interscience. East, R., & Uncles, M.D. (2008). In praise of retrospective surveys. Journal of Marketing Management, 24(9/10), 929–944. Ellis, P.D. (2008). Does psychic distance moderate the market-size entry sequence relationship? Journal of International Business Studies, 39(3), 351–369. Evangelista, F., Albaum, G., & Poon, P. (1999). An empirical test of alternative theories of survey response behaviour. Journal of the Market Research Society, 41(2), 227–244. Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Ford, D. (1973). Commitment: A mathematical model. Quality and Quantity, 7, 1–40. Furse, D.H., & Stewart, D. (1984). Manipulating dissonance to improve mail survey response. Psychology and Marketing, 1, 79–94. Furse, D.H., Stewart, D., & Rados, D. (1981). Effects of foot-in-the-door cash incentives and follow-ups on survey response. Journal of Marketing Research, 19, 473–478. Gendall, P., Hoek, J., & Esselmont, D. (1995). The effect of appeal, complexity, and tone in a mail survey covering letter. Journal of the Market Research Society, 37(3), 251–266. Geurts, M., O’Neill, M., Lawrence, K., & Albaum, G. (1988). Increasing response rates using an inducement question in mail surveys. Journal of Direct Marketing, 2, 35–42. Groves, R.M., Cialdini, R.B., & Couper, M.P. (1992). Understanding the decision to participate in a survey. Public Opinion Quarterly, 56, 475–495. Groves, R.M., & Couper, M.P. (1998). Non-response in household interview surveys. New York: Wiley. Hackler, J., & Bourgette, P. (1973). Dollars, dissonance and survey returns. Public Opinion Quarterly, 37, 276–281. Hagger, M., Biddle, S., Chow, E., Stambulova, N., & Kavussanu, M. (2003). Physical self-perceptions in adolescence: Generalizability of a hierarchical multidimensional model across three cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 34(6), 611–628. 1188 Journal of Marketing Management, Volume 28 Harzing, A.-W. (2006). Response styles in cross-national survey research: A 26-country study. International Journal of Cross-Cultural Management, 6(2), 243–266. Havitz, M.E., & Howard, D.R. (1995). How enduring is enduring involvement? A seasonal examination of three recreational activities. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 4(3), 255–276. Helgeson, J.G, Voss, K.E., & Terpening, W.D. (2002). Determinants of mail-survey response: Survey design factors and respondent factors. Psychology and Marketing, 19(3), 303–328. Hofstede, G. (2001). Culture’s consequences: Comparing values, behaviors, institutions, and organizations across nations (2nd ed.). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Hornback, K. (1971). Toward a theory of involvement propensity for collective behaviour. Sociological Forces, 4, 61–77. Houston, M.L., & Rothschild, M.L. (1978). Conceptual and methodological perspectives on involvement. In S.C. Jain (Ed.), Research frontiers in marketing: Dialogues and directions (pp. 184–187). Chicago: American Marketing Association. Johnson, T.P., & Van de Vijver, F.J.R. (2003). Social desirability in cross-cultural research. In J. Harkness, F.J.R. Van de Vijver, & P.Ph. Mohler (Eds.), Cross-cultural survey methods. New York: Wiley. Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decisions under risk. Econometrica, 47, 263–291. Kogut, B., & Singh, H. (1988). The effect of national culture on the choice of entry mode. Journal of International Business Studies, 19(3), 411–432. Lyness, K.S., & Kropf, M.B. (2007). Cultural values and potential nonresponse bias: A multilevel examination of cross-national differences in mail survey response rates. Organizational Research Methods, 10(2), 210–224. Lynn, P. (2001). The impact of incentives on response rates to personal interview surveys. International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 13(3), 326–337. Martin, W.C., Duncan, W.J., & Powers, T.L. (1989). Costs and benefits of selected response inducement techniques in mail survey research. Journal of Business Research, 19(1), 67–79. Poon, P., Albaum, G.,&Evangelista, F. (2003).Why people respond to surveys: A theory based study of Hong Kong respondents. Journal of International Consumer Marketing, 16(2), 75–90. Powers, T.L., & Valentine, D.B. (2009). Response quality in consumer satisfaction research. Journal of Consumer Marketing, 26(4), 232–240. Roster, C.A., Rogers, R.D., Hozier, G.C., Jr., Baker, K.G., & Albaum, G. (2007).Management of marketing research projects: Does delivery method matter anymore in survey research? Journal of Marketing Theory and Practice, 15(2), 127–145. Shaw, M., Bednall, D., & Hall, J. (2002). A proposal for a comprehensive response-rate measure (CRRM) for survey research. Journal of Marketing Management, 18(5), 533–554. Shimp, T.A., & Sharma, S. (1987). Consumer ethnocentrism: Construction and validation of the CETSCALE. Journal of Marketing Research, 24(3), 280–289. Skjak, K.K., & Harkness, J. (2003). Data collection methods. In J. Harkness, F.J.R. Van de Vijver, & P.Ph. Mohler (Eds.), Cross-cultural survey methods. New York: Wiley. Sousa, C.M.P., & Bradley, F. (2006). Cultural distance and psychic distance: Two peas in a pod? Journal of International Marketing, 14(1), 49–70. Sousa, C.M.P., & Bradley, F. (2008). Cultural distance and psychic distance: Refinements in conceptualisation and measurement. Journal of International Management, 24(5–6), 467–488. Tourangeau, R., & Ye, C. (2009). The framing of the survey request and panel attrition. Public Opinion Quarterly, 73(2), 338–348. Tullar, W., Pressley, M., & Gentry, D. (1979). Toward a theoretical framework for mail survey response. Proceedings of the Third Annual Conference of the Academy of Marketing Science (pp. 243–247). Miami, FL: Academy of Marketing Science. Evangelista et al. Using response behaviour theory to solicit survey participation 1189 Tybout, A., & Yalch, R. (1980). The effect of experience: A matter of salience? Journal of Consumer Research, 7, 406–413. Watkins, D., Adair, J., Akande, A., Gerong, A., McInerny, D., Sunar, D., Watson, S., Wen, Q., & Wondimer, H. (1998). Individualism-collectivism, gender, and self-concept: A nine culture investigation. Psychologia, 41, 259–271. Webster, C. (1997). Effects of researcher presence and appeal on response quality in handdelivered, self-administered surveys. Journal of Business Research, 38(2), 105–114. About the authors Felicitas Evangelista is associate professor at the School of Marketing, University of Western Sydney. Her teaching and research activities have been in the areas of research methods, marketing, and international business. She has published in scholarly journals such as the Journal of Business Research, Journal of World Business, Management Science, and International Marketing Review. She has served as external assessor of research grants and has served as a reviewer for reputable journals. T 61-2-9685-9687 E F.Evangelista@uws.edu.au Patrick Poon is currently the head and an associate professor in the Department of Marketing and International Business, Lingnan University, Hong Kong. He received his Master of Engineering and Technology Management and PhD (Technology Management and Marketing) from the University of Queensland. His research interests include technology management, cross-cultural consumer studies, services marketing, knowledge management, and social marketing. He has published in various international referred journals. T 852-2616-8235 E patpoon@ln.edu.hk Gerald Albaum is research professor at the Robert O. Anderson Schools of Management at the University of New Mexico. In addition, he holds an appointment as Senior Research Fellow at the IC2 Institute, University of Texas at Austin, and is Professor Emeritus of Marketing at the University of Oregon. He received his PhD in 1962 from the University of Wisconsin–Madison, and his MBA (1958) and BA (1954) from the University of Washington. He is the author or co-author of eight books, more than 90 articles in refereed journals, 40 papers in refereed conference proceedings, and 20 papers in other publications. His writings deal with issues in research methods, international marketing activities, cross-cultural/national research, and retailing (especially direct selling).Many of his studies are cross-cultural/national in nature. Corresponding author: Gerald Albaum, Anderson School of Management, MSC05 3090, 1 University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM 87131-0001, USA. T 1-505-277-2437 E albaum@mgt.unm.edu Copyright of Journal of Marketing Management is the property of Routledge and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.

IS IT YOUR FIRST TIME HERE? WELCOME

USE COUPON "11OFF" AND GET 11% OFF YOUR ORDERS